New research suggests that it is prudent to think twice before
allowing people who know each other to be in positions to judge each
other."In judging people we already know, we are more or less unable to
ignore our previously established images of those people," says Daniel
Leising of Technische Universität Dresden. The new study, published
today in Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, examines how
real people evaluate the behavior of themselves, their friends, and
strangers. Psychologists know that people hold a number of biases when
evaluating others, but most studies to date on this issue have used
written descriptions of the behavior of hypothetical persons. "This is
one of the few studies that investigated judgments of people's actual
behavior," Leising says. Leising and colleagues recruited pairs of friends for the study, asking
them first to describe each others' personalities and then several days
later, videotaped them participating in standardized, challenging
situations in the lab. The tasks ranged from answering general knowledge
questions, such as "How high is Mount Everest?," to a role-playing
exercise in which participants had to call a "neighbor" (played by an
actor) and demand that she turn down the volume on her stereo, to
telling a joke of his or her own choice. The participants, their
friends, and strangers then evaluated the videotapes, each about 90
seconds long. "This way, we could compare different views on the exact same behaviors
with one another," Leising explains. "If different people watch the
exact same videotapes but interpret them differently, then the different
interpretations may not be rooted in what they just saw, but must be
explained in terms of something else." The research team found that they could predict how participants would
judge their friends' behavior based on what they thought of them in
advance, even before watching their videotaped behavior. "By
statistically controlling for strangers' ratings of the same behavior,
we could show that there are two kinds of systematic bias in such
behavior judgments," Leising says. First, we judge the behavior of people we know in ways that are
consistent with our general attitude toward them, so we attribute
positive qualities to the behavior of people we like. Also, we judge
people we know to match our specific impressions of them: For example,
if we think of someone as being generally talkative, we will judge that
person to be more talkative in specific situations beyond what a
stranger would see in the very same behavior. "We really like to have our images of persons be consistent," Leising
says. "This is probably beneficial in terms of arriving at an overall
image that is representative - for example, if the person's behavior in a
situation is very atypical, we could discount it as an exception and
not let it influence our overall image of the person much," Leising
says. That representative image then allows us to predict people's
future behavior. Additionally, he says that the tendency to idealize our
friends might serve as a "social glue" that increases social cohesion.
"In our evolutionary past, that probably constituted a major advantage
in terms of survival." But the flip side, Leising says, is that in specific situations, we are
not able to objectively evaluate people we know, which could be
problematic, say, in a classroom. "For example, a professor who
considers his student to be highly intelligent will probably tend to
overestimate that student's performance in an oral exam," he says. "As
long as all students are treated that way, it will be OK. But the more
likely case is that the professor will not have identical images of all
of his students and judge them accordingly." Conducting anonymous
assessments is preferable when possible, as well as ensuring that
performances are assessed by people who have no prior acquaintance with
those they are judging. While the new study asked participants to make their judgments using
everyday terms and language, Leising would like to see future such
studies examine more factual qualities of behavior - for example, in
eyewitness testimony, asking which person was the first to physically
attack the other person. He also would like to see future work explore
more negative attributes. But recruiting people who know but do not like
each other is a research challenge. "We're still working on ways to overcome that difficulty," Leising say.
"Including critical informants in future studies is important, because
the social world we inhabit often comprises not only friends, but also
foes. In my view, that aspect is yet insufficiently represented in
present-day personality research."
Source: Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin
Source: Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin
No comments:
Post a Comment